Money matters to happiness--perhaps more than previously thought (2024)

What's the relationship between money and well-being? "It's one of the most studied questions in my field," says Matthew Killingsworth, a senior fellow at Penn's Wharton School who studies human happiness. "I'm very curious about it. Other scientists are curious about it. Laypeople are curious about it. It's something everyone is navigating all the time."

To answer this question, Killingsworth collected 1.7 million data points from more than 33,000 participants who provided in-the-moment snapshots of their feelings during daily life. In a paper in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Killingsworth confirms that money does influence happiness and, contrary to previous influential research on the subject suggesting that this plateaus above $75,000, there was no dollar value at which it stopped mattering to an individual's well-being.

Killingsworth conducts much of his work using a technique called experience sampling, which asks people to repeatedly fill out short surveys at randomly selected moments during their day. "It tells us what's actually happening in people's real lives as they live them, in millions of moments as they work and chat and eat and watch TV."

Most previous studies of the money-happiness link focused on evaluative well-being, which encompasses overall satisfaction with life. But for this study, Killingsworth aimed to capture both evaluative and experienced well-being, the latter indicating how people feel in the moment.

Through an app he created called Track Your Happiness, people recorded this a few times each day, with check-in times randomized per participant. To measure experienced well-being, each check-in asked them, "How do you feel right now?" on a scale ranging from "very bad" to "very good." At least once during the process, participants also answered the question, "Overall, how satisfied are you with your life?" on a scale of "not at all" to "extremely." This measured evaluative well-being.

Secondary measures of experienced well-being included 12 specific feelings, five positive (confident, good, inspired, interested, and proud) and seven negative (afraid, angry, bad, bored, sad, stressed, and upset). Secondary measures of evaluative well-being included two other measures of life satisfaction collected on an intake survey.

"This process provided repeated snapshots of people's lives, which collectively gives us a composite image, a stop-motion movie of their lives," he says. In total, 33,391 employed, 18- to 65-year-olds in the United States provided 1,725,994 reports of experienced well-being. "Scientists often talk about trying to get a representative sample of the population," he adds. "I was trying to get a representative sample of the moments of people's lives."

Killingsworth then calculated the average level of well-being for each person and analyzed its relationship to people's income. In part, he was trying to confirm the findings of a 2010 paper that suggested that as people earn more money their well-being increases, but experienced well-being plateaus once annual household income hits $75,000.

"It's a compelling possibility, the idea that money stops mattering above that point, at least for how people actually feel moment to moment," he says. "But when I looked across a wide range of income levels, I found that all forms of well-being continued to rise with income. I don't see any sort of kink in the curve, an inflection point where money stops mattering. Instead, it keeps increasing."

Here, "income" refers to a concept known as log(income); rather than each dollar mattering the same to each person, each dollar starts to matter less the more a person earns. "We would expect two people earning $25,000 and $50,000, respectively, to have the same difference in well-being as two people earning $100,000 and $200,000, respectively. In other words, proportional differences in income matter the same to everyone."

Beyond that, Killingsworth's work also provides a deeper understanding of the link between income and happiness.

Higher earners are happier, in part, because of an increased sense of control over life, he says. "When you have more money, you have more choices about how to live your life. You can likely see this in the pandemic. People living paycheck to paycheck who lose their job might need to take the first available job to stay afloat, even if it's one they dislike. People with a financial cushion can wait for one that's a better fit. Across decisions big and small, having more money gives a person more choices and a greater sense of autonomy."

Yet it might be best not to define success in monetary terms, he says. "Although money might be good for happiness, I found that people who equated money and success were less happy than those who didn't. I also found that people who earned more money worked longer hours and felt more pressed for time."

Though the study does show that income matters beyond a previously believed threshold, Killingsworth also doesn't want the takeaway to enforce an idea that people should focus more on money. In fact, he found that, in actuality, income is only a modest determinant of happiness.

"If anything, people probably overemphasize money when they think about how well their life is going," says Killingsworth. "Yes, this is a factor that might matter in a way that we didn't fully realize before, but it's just one of many that people can control and ultimately, it's not one I'm terribly concerned people are undervaluing." Rather, he says he hopes this research can help move forward the conversation in an attempt to find what he calls the "equation for human happiness."

Matthew Killingsworth is a senior fellow in Wharton People Analytics in the Wharton School and an associate in MindCORE in the School of Arts & Sciences at the University of Pennsylvania.

Matthew Killingsworth's work on the relationship between money and well-being stands as a cornerstone in the field of happiness research. His study, published in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, drew from a substantial dataset of 1.7 million data points collected from over 33,000 participants. He utilized a unique method called experience sampling, where individuals reported their in-the-moment feelings multiple times a day through an app named Track Your Happiness.

Killingsworth's research expanded beyond the previous focus on evaluative well-being to encompass both evaluative and experienced well-being. By capturing immediate emotions and overall life satisfaction, he created a comprehensive picture of individuals' happiness in various moments. This study included detailed measures of specific feelings, both positive and negative, to paint a nuanced understanding of human well-being.

One of the key findings that deviated from prior research was the debunking of the notion that happiness plateaus after an annual household income of $75,000. Contrary to this belief, Killingsworth's analysis revealed a continuous increase in well-being with rising income, without a clear saturation point. He emphasized the significance of proportional differences in income rather than absolute values, suggesting that each additional dollar matters less as income grows.

Moreover, Killingsworth's research shed light on the correlation between higher income and happiness, attributing a portion of increased happiness among higher earners to a heightened sense of control over life choices. He pointed out the flexibility and autonomy that accompany financial stability, enabling individuals to make decisions aligned with their preferences and values, especially evident during challenges like the pandemic.

However, he cautioned against equating success solely with monetary wealth, highlighting that those who correlated money and success tended to report lower levels of happiness. Additionally, his findings indicated that higher earners often worked longer hours and felt more time-constrained, hinting at potential trade-offs associated with increased income.

Killingsworth's study serves as a reminder not to overly prioritize money when evaluating one's life satisfaction. While income does play a role, it's just one among numerous factors influencing happiness that individuals can control. He emphasizes the need to continue exploring the multifaceted aspects contributing to human happiness, aiming to create a holistic "equation for human happiness."

Matthew Killingsworth, a senior fellow at Penn's Wharton School and an associate in MindCORE at the University of Pennsylvania, has significantly contributed to understanding the intricate relationship between money and well-being through extensive empirical research employing innovative methodologies in the field of happiness studies.

Money matters to happiness--perhaps more than previously thought (2024)
Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Rev. Leonie Wyman

Last Updated:

Views: 5649

Rating: 4.9 / 5 (79 voted)

Reviews: 94% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Rev. Leonie Wyman

Birthday: 1993-07-01

Address: Suite 763 6272 Lang Bypass, New Xochitlport, VT 72704-3308

Phone: +22014484519944

Job: Banking Officer

Hobby: Sailing, Gaming, Basketball, Calligraphy, Mycology, Astronomy, Juggling

Introduction: My name is Rev. Leonie Wyman, I am a colorful, tasty, splendid, fair, witty, gorgeous, splendid person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.